What kind of career can I have by studying animal behaviour and cognition ?

The short answer? All kinds of things!!

Have you ever considered studying in a field of animal behaviour and wondered what kind of careers are possible following your studies? Or are you a current student in an area related to animal behaviour, welfare, psychology, cognition or human evolution interested in what options might be out there for you?

It is often a challenging field to break into, and not everyone decides to stay in academia or directly continue studying animal behaviour! But everyone is in agreement that the transferable skills learned from hands on experience working in animal behaviour, cognition and welfare research provides our researchers with many career options in the professional world. We caught up with our Living Links (LL) and Budongo Research Unit (BRU) alumni to catch up with past researchers to find out what they are doing now for their careers! From interns and research assistants who have supported the research here, to MSc and PhD students who conducted their thesis research at LL & BRU, and to the more experienced Postdoctoral researchers, all of them have gained skills and experience from working in this field that has taken them to their chosen career paths now and beyond.

From academic publishing companies, dog welfare charities, University lecturers, conservation and biodiversity management, research group leaders, training search and rescue rats, farming, filming, wildlife writing, Human Resources and continued research into animal and human minds, communication and welfare, the different roles are fascinating and very varied! Read on to see what our Alumni are doing now!

Christoph Völter: Research group leader / senior scientist at MPI-EVA (Leipzig) and Vetmeduni Vienna

I’m leading the Comparative Cognition research group within the department of Comparative Cultural Psychology at MPI-EVA (with a focus on great ape cognition). At the same time, I’m the PI of two research projects on canine cognition (with a focus on eye-tracking studies) at the Messerli Research Institute, Vetmeduni Vienna.

Zeynep Civelek: Research Integrity Specialist, Frontiers

After leaving academia, I sought a role that remained connected to science without focusing on research. This led me to a career as a specialist in scientific publishing, where I now contribute to advancing research from a different perspective. The open-access publication process involves specialised teams working collaboratively to make research available for everyone. My role involves ensuring adherence to ethical and publication standards in submissions to uphold the integrity of academic research. Additionally I collaborate with editors to investigate post-publication issues. I work remotely with an amazing and dedicated team of experts!

Elizabeth Warren: Postdoctoral research fellow at Johns Hopkins University

I worked on research with squirrel monkeys, capuchins and chimpanzees at Living Links and BRU (MSc/PhD). In my postdoc position now, I do research with great apes and with dogs, exploring questions like what they think about when they communicate, and whether they can make better decisions as groups than as individuals. I do my primate work in zoos like Maryland Zoo in Baltimore and Indianapolis Zoo, as well as Ngamba Chimpanzee Sanctuary in Uganda. With dogs, we work with the pets of people in our community right here in Baltimore! My supervisor is Chris Krupenye who is also BRU Alumni (see below!).

Shreejata Gupta: Postdoc at Institute for Language, Communication and the Brain, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France

Having worked with monkeys and apes, examining their gestural communication, I acquired observational and experimental skills in ethology and comparative cognition. Using what I learnt from observing primates in the wild and captivity, I now apply the same methods to investigate preverbal gestures in human infants and test whether they are precursors to the linguistic co-speech gestures in human adults. Additionally, using tools like eye tracking (something I learnt with the chimps at Budongo trail) and neuroimaging techniques (EEG+fNIRS) I am looking at how infants develop their capacities of perception and comprehension of socially produced gestures.

Luke Townrow: PhD candidate at Johns Hopkins University (USA)

I first came to Edinburgh Zoo to work an internship with Professor Josep Call during my psychology undergraduate degree at Cardiff University. I stayed at the zoo for little under a year (cut short by COVID) but I managed to work for and learn from an incredible range of professors and research staff. I had opportunities to work with the chimpanzees, capuchin monkeys, squirrel monkeys and geladas! I met amazing people who I would later work for including Professor Katie Slocombe, before completing my masters with Professor Zanna Clay at Durham University on human empathy and perspective-taking. I am now a 3rd year PhD student at Johns Hopkins University in Maryland, USA under the mentorship of Dr. Christopher Krupenye (below!) studying human and nonhuman primate theory of mind and social cognition. 

Elle Jordan: Data Science and Analytics Manager, Dogs Trust (charity sector)

I worked at Living Links & Budongo both for my PhD research with capuchins, and as a keeper. Following my PhD, finding a career where I could use my research skills to positively impact animal welfare led me to the Dogs Trust. In my current role I lead a team of scientists who are developing digitised operational systems and reporting tools to improve dog welfare in rehoming centres. Alongside this I also work on various dog-related research projects ranging from investigating dog behaviour and health post-adoption, to understanding what motivates individuals to adopt dogs.

Christopher Krupenye: Assistant Professor of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University

In my current role, I am fortunate to be able to continue research that is similar in nature to work I did at BRU but now including humans and pet dogs in addition to apes. I also teach a couple classes on the minds of infants and animals, mentor postdocs and postgraduate and undergraduate students on their research, and contribute various forms of service to my department, university, and the field. As one example, I am now an Associate Editor at an academic journal, Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

Donna Kean: PostDoc Researcher, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona

After completing my PhD in primate cognition, I went on to work as a Behavioural Research Scientist at an NGO called APOPO, based in Tanzania, where I was training African giant pouched rats to do search and rescue, to detect disease, and to identify soil contamination for environmental clean-up projects!

Following this, I now work as a Post-doctoral Researcher at the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, working in a team to build ‘Rat Villages’ used to conduct automated touchscreen behavioural tasks researching the evolution of cognition and welfare.

Abi Gwynn: Market Gardener at Soul Farm, previously role Programme Development and Liaison Officer at Borneo Nature Foundation.

Abi was a Research Assistant at Living Links working with the capuchin monkeys on a working memory experiment. She later studied for her Masters at Exeter University and conducted research on monitoring GI parasites in wild orang-utans and how this can help inform conservation. This led her to a previous role with the Borneo Nature Foundation as a Programme Development and Liaison Officer. Continuing in the nature industry Abi has now moved on to another passion of hers and works in the farming sector.

Matthias Allritz: PostDoc Researcher, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany

After working many years with the chimpanzees in the Budongo Research Unit I have moved back to the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology and I am carrying out studies on cognition in all four great ape species at Leipzig Zoo, gorillas, orangutans, bonobos and of course chimpanzees! I am currently working on studies regarding communication and spatial cognition.

Anna Redly: Research Assistant (ADHD Research), Kings College London

I have a background in Neuroscience (BSc) and Comparative & Evolutionary Psychology (MSc). In my time at Living Links I really enjoyed working in research and public engagement and running an experiment with the capuchins, but realised after that mental health is my true passion! I worked for a year in the SEN department at a secondary school and am now a research assistant on a project about ADHD in adolescents.

Sophia Daoudi-Simison: Animal Behaviour Lecturer, Newcastle University

I have a permanent teaching position at Newcastle University which has allowed me to continue my research in primate behaviour and welfare. Having previously worked as a research assistant and student at Living Links, it’s quite a change to now be supervising my own projects. I’ve broadened my research to include geladas, marmosets, and chimpanzees, building on my earlier work with capuchins, squirrel monkeys, and lemurs. In addition to research in zoos, my work has taken me to the field in countries such as Suriname, Peru, and Costa Rica, enhancing my understanding of primate behaviour in various contexts. I also collaborate with Living Links on citizen science initiatives. I’m committed to advancing our knowledge in the field, particularly in ecologically valid methods that consider the behavioural ecology of the study species.

Leoma Williams: Biodiversity Consultant & Freelance writer for BBC Wildlife Magazine

I spent many cherished years working with the Edinburgh chimpanzees (MSc/PhD), as well as other intelligent animals such as ravens. Leaving academia I was very keen to find a job that allowed me to continue working with and for animals, in a way that felt practical and important. I now work as a biodiversity specialist for a large environmental consultancy. This involves working with international clients to help projects – be that infrastructure, energy, or resource extraction – to comply with environmental regulations, to ensure that negative impacts on biodiversity are avoided, minimised, mitigated, or compensated for. I use a lot of the skills I gained in academia in this role – analysing literature to gather important species data, writing clear and accurate reports,  presenting data, as well as fieldwork to survey animal populations.

I am also very lucky in that I am able to utilise my chimpanzee knowledge gained at Edinburgh Zoo in this role, as some projects involve analysing risks to wild chimpanzees and may involve conducting chimpanzee surveys in Africa in the future! In addition to this FT role I also write regular columns for BBC Wildlife Magazine on various nature topics, something I have been doing for several years and am happy to be able to keep as a fun sideline. I have a life-long love for animals (as can be seen from my wedding photo from this summer!) and  I feel very grateful to be able to talk, write, and think about animals every day, even outside of academia.

Amy Derrick: H&S advisor, Royal Society for the Protection of Birds, nature conservation charity

I was at Living Links for a few months working as an intern research assistant for Donna Kean above, carrying out research with the capuchin monkeys. My plan was to find further research positions in the future, but after a bit of soul searching and working in the gardening industry, I realised that wasn’t the right path for me. However I knew I still wanted to stay somewhere within the natural world/research/conservation industry and so eventually found myself in the RSPB. Not in a direct conservation role, but instead working as a H&S advisor which involves me supporting the people and the work being carried out for essential conservation work in Scotland, which allows me to still feel satisfied that I’m doing my bit! I get to work at some beautiful locations, like the RSPB Abernethy Reserve pictured below!

“Busy Boards” as primate enrichment

Hello, my name is Nailah and I am an Experienced Animal Keeper on the Budongo/Living Links section at RZSS Edinburgh Zoo. As a keeper looking after the research monkeys at Living Links, part of our role is helping to enhance animal welfare by proving stimulating and exciting enrichment for the monkey groups. Usually this is food-based enrichment hidden in objects, often challenging for the monkeys to extract so that they have to use their minds to get the food out! However, other types of enrichment can be made for the monkeys that is not focused on food- I have been working on designing and creating a new enrichment for the Living Links squirrel monkeys called a Busy Board

Nailah busy creating her boards from mixed materials!

For some time now, busy boards have been designed and implemented with human toddlers in mind. The idea behind a busy board is for children to be exposed to a host of different textures, noises, colours and unusual items all part of one item to play with. Even though they were initially intended for children, the advantages mentioned below are also beneficial to non-human primates. I wanted to recreate an interactive activity board that I had previously made for other species of primates for the monkeys at Living Links. I attached various items on this board that food can be hidden in for the monkeys, such as square knots made out of old firehose, pieces of hose pipe, brush heads and even old socks!

Pictured here is the West group of squirrel monkeys having their first look at their new busy board!

So what are the benefits of a busy board?

  • It may facilitate the development of brain neurons. Sensory play aids in the physical growth of the brain and strengthens the pathways between neurons in the brain
  • Sensory play is closely linked to cognitive development, problem-solving abilities, and fine and gross motor skills
  • Sensory play enhances an individual’s memory through practice. By interacting with different textures, tastes, and smells, primates use their senses to strengthen and build their cognitive and motor memories
  • Every time a primate touches an object that is cold, sticky, wet, or has different textures, they are expanding on their prior knowledge of these properties.
  • Through sensory play, primates can associate new objects to similar ones they have already encountered by using their brain’s ability to recognise characteristics in objects and sensations. We have had researchers test this in our monkeys in past research projects!
Squirrel monkeys in their outdoor enclosure exploring new objects in a past research study.

Designing experimental apparatus for monkeys!

Written by Dr Emma McEwen

In many of the experiments at Living Links we use physical apparatuses that the monkeys can interact with, like different puzzle boxes, enrichment containers, dispenser shoots or objects for them to explore. When we design these new apparatuses, there are lots of different considerations we have to be aware of, and its often a collaborative effort between the researcher, the keeper team, and our Psychology & Neuroscience workshop to come up with the best solutions!

Our primary concern is always the safety of the monkeys, and of the experimenters. We always have to consider that a material is safe for the monkeys to touch in a way that they can’t get hurt, is non-toxic and easily cleaned so that it’s safe for their food rewards to touch, and something they can’t pull into their enclosure and do damage with.

Some design process stages including workshop cutting of materials, sketches and mock up protocol

Secondly, it has to be durable – capuchin monkeys can be stronger that you might think! We always have to consider the durability of the materials we use, making sure they can’t be easily torn or snapped, and that the monkeys can’t knock them off the experimenter’s table.

It also has to be enjoyable – we always want our research sessions to be enjoyable experiences and only positively reinforce behaviour. This includes only using materials and apparatuses that the capuchins enjoy interacting with. Sometimes, it can be difficult to predict whether they will enjoy touching certain materials. For this reason, we sometimes run some taster sessions, called pilot sessions, to offer the monkeys a chance to interact with a material or part of an apparatus before we build the finished product.

Two types of flap material (green plastic and black silicone) and a monkey reaching into the flap box for a reward!

For example, myself and my collaborator Andreea Miscov are currently testing two types of material to be used as potential ‘flaps’ for a grid box the monkeys will need to search for food rewards in, as part of a memory experiment. One is a silicone material and the other a harder plastic material. There are often individual differences between monkeys, and while some of the monkeys have no problem touching the silicone material, others have not wanted to! On the other hand the plastic material seems preferred from pilot testing so far. It can be important that there are no major differences between how the monkeys interact with apparatus, especially with experiments looking at time measures (a monkey less keen to touch an area of the apparatus will take longer to get their food reward!).

Video example showing durability test, prototype flap box and piloting with the monkeys!

Last but not least, it must be practical – for the designs of our experiments, we need our apparatuses to function in particular ways such as hiding food, sliding things into other things, moving in certain ways, or having certain weights. It can sometimes take us lots of time and trips to hardware shops to browse different materials and find the one that functions or moves in the exact way we’ve dreamed of when designing the experiment!

It can be very challenging to find something that fits all these criteria, but it’s a fun problem to solve!

Squirrel Monkey Training at Living Links

Written by Kenna Valles, Keeper, RZSS Edinburgh Zoo

Training in zoos provides several benefits to the animals and keepers. Training helps to keep animals both physically and mentally stimulated, while building a positive relationship between the keepers and animals, increasing the trust and communication between the two and encouraging animals to voluntarily participate in veterinary and husbandry procedures. This both builds the animal’s confidence and reduces their stress levels during these necessary events.

Figure 1: Squirrel monkeys playing in their enclosure

Just like for your pets at home, veterinary procedures in particular can be very stressful for monkeys; they often have to be separated from their group in a smaller area of the enclosure, sometimes physically restrained and handled by the keepers, without an understanding of why. However, to keep our animals healthy, these procedures are necessary to carry out regularly. Specifically training animals for veterinary purposes decreases an animal’s fear towards these procedures, allowing them to cooperate in their veterinary care and associate the procedure with a positive outcome by giving them a reward for participating.

The training performed at Living Links by keepers uses exclusively positive reinforcement techniques. This means that the training is voluntary to participate in and the monkeys are given a valuable reward, such as a food item, for performing a desired behaviour, without any punishment for not performing a behaviour. It is also perfectly acceptable for an animal to choose not to engage in training and their daily routine will continue as normal. Our research activities followed a similar training strategy, with the monkeys developing over the years a strong positive association with our research cubicles, the fun cognitively enriching games, and very tasty food rewards! Monkeys voluntarily come into the cubicles to take part as they find the experience so positively rewarding, and it’s this that keeps them returning each day for research.

Figure 2: Dominant male Boa from the ‘East’ squirrel monkey group taking part in scale training

The keepers have been training the monkeys to have this positive association with various veterinary procedures, such as voluntarily sitting on a weighing scale to check body weight (one important indicator of overall health) and entering an area of their enclosure called the ‘holding area’ which is used for veterinary checks and procedures.

To encourage the monkeys to start coming into the holding area and build a positive association with it, keepers rewarded each monkey for coming in by giving them a raisin, a highly valued food reward. When the training first started, the “West” monkeys were very nervous coming into the holding area; only around eight monkeys would come in for a raisin and they would then immediately run back out again. However over several training sessions, the monkeys started to become more confident and trusting of coming into the holding area and soon all 16 “West” monkeys were coming in for raisins! They also became more comfortable staying in the holding area for longer periods of time and now most monkeys will stay there eating for several minutes before leaving.

Figure 3: Keeper Kenna Valles training the squirrel monkeys in the holding area

Prior to this training, when the monkeys were closed in the holding area for procedures, they would be visibly stressed and huddle in a large group in a corner. During the annual vaccinations a year ago, only one ‘West’ squirrel monkeys voluntarily entered the holding area, but this year, after all the hard work with training, all 16 monkeys chose to come in by themselves! In addition to this they were not huddled together and instead were wandering around the holding area and continued to come to the keepers for raisins, suggesting they were significantly more comfortable being in the area than previous years. This is a really great success for both keeper and monkeys!

The “East” squirrel monkey group have also been participating in the same training but have a bit more work to do before they reach the same comfort level as the ‘West’ group, but the keepers are confident they will get there! It is still stressful for the monkeys when the actual procedures are carried out (i.e. injections, being weighed) but Kenna and the keeping team hope to work on training them for these procedures as well, and hopefully with enough training, the monkeys (both our squirrel monkeys and our capuchin monkeys) will eventually be comfortable participating in any veterinary procedure.

New Look Living Links!

Some of our regular visitors may have noticed that Living Links has recently undergone a change in appearance- after 15 years it felt time for a refresh! Led by Research Fellow Anna Redly, Director Professor Amanda Seed and RZSS Interpretation officer Krystyna Keir, the RZSS and University of St Andrews teams have worked closely together to design and implement a fresh, fun and colourful new look to Living Links.

While the work is ongoing and covers several phases, visitors can already enjoy the benefits of the new look Living Links so far! New flags lead the way up the walkways to Living Links, highlighting some of the similarities between us and non-human primates; problem solving, learning, curiosity, playfulness. The different careers supporting the running of Living Links, from keepers to researchers to technicians (and YOU!), are highlighted when you first reach the outer balconies, and will be a feature we focus on in the new Living Links theme.

Our bright new fun colour scheme can be seen throughout the building, inside and out. In front of the windows of the monkeys indoor enclosures we have spinning cubes and panels for visitors to meet some of our monkey faces, learn a bit about them and try to ID them through the window! These have proved extremely popular with children, so much so that we had to install quieter spin systems as the constant use of them was slightly disturbing the monkeys during research!

Outside there are information flaps on monkey behaviour, the ever popular primate family tree with linked information, and soon we will have some behaviour spotting panels and QR links to online monkey ID guides. We will have students running occasional citizen science research from here, asking for your help with one of our long running welfare studies, and we hope this popular task will run more regularly in 2024.

Exiting the exhibit on either side, the flags display a ‘did you spot us…’ examples of behaviours you may see from the monkeys at Living Links- and if you didn’t see it on this visit, keep an eye out next time you are at Edinburgh Zoo!

There are a number of new features still to come later this month, including a ‘join the team’ face cut out board and a special ‘Living Links through time’ highlighting the key people and research themes that have featured at Living Links over the past 15 years. We will also be gradually developing interactive citizen science games for a large screen that will be part of the Living Links timeline, highlighting the importance of getting YOU involved in future research!

There is room for further development with visitor feedback, so we would love to hear from you if you have any particular ideas of what you would like to see or do at Living Links and if you would be interested in being more involved with everyday science! Contact us at livinglinks@st-andrews.ac.uk with your input or join our newsletter to be kept up-to-date!

First Impressions- a research intern’s view

Written by: Wilson Chen

Hello, and welcome to the Living Links Blog! My name is Wilson, I am a second-year psychology student at the University of St Andrews. I am currently working in a public engagement with research internship at Living Links. I was interested in an internship at Living Links because I wanted to learn more about the role of monkeys in the study of behaviour and cognition. Moreover, I believe that bridging the gap between psychology research at Living Links and the visitors at Edinburgh Zoo is a meaningful endeavour that can promote and educate the public about psychology as a scientific subject.

Starting a new internship in a zoo can be both exciting and nerve-wracking. I may have been looking forward to this opportunity for a long time, but it can be hard to know what to expect when you’re stepping into a new environment with new people and animals. In this blog post, I will talk about my first impression of the centre and share some of my experiences talking to visitors during my first weekend as an intern.

            Originally, I thought the first thing I was likely to notice was the smell. The smell of monkey enclosures can be quite striking, as primates have strong body odour and produce a lot of waste. However, I was pleasantly surprised at how everything is clean and tidy at Living Links. The environment on the main platform (which is where I spend most of my time working) is fantastic. I can get a great view of both the east wing and the west wing enclosures. It can get quite chilly sometimes, especially when the wind picks up, but I am sure the weather will be much more pleasant when spring comes around.

            Whether it’s to see the exotic animals or to learn about wildlife conservation efforts, visitors often come with their own expectations and interests, and it is very apparent that most visitors are very engaged with the experience Living Links has to offer. When I approached visitors about doing a visitor feedback survey, many visitors reacted positively. To be completely honest, I thought I was going to get shut down most of the time, but I was happy to be proven wrong. Visitors of all ages can be seen at Living Links, and we see a lot of families. For the children coming with their parents, this could well be their first time learning about scientific research. The Living Links centre could be a great starting point in children’s engagement with scientific inquiry and the nature of science.

            While walking around the place asking people to fill in surveys about visitor experiences, I get a lot of opportunities to watch the capuchin monkeys go about their day. As I started to observe them, I began seeing a lot of similarities between their behaviour and human behaviour. Capuchins are social animals that form complex relationships and have a wide range of emotions. Watching them interact with each other can be very fascinating and heart-warming. You may also be struck by how intelligent they are, as you observe them playing, using tools, and communicating with each other in sophisticated ways. One thing that stood out to me is the similarity between the dietary habits of capuchin monkeys and human beings and how they do a lot of the same things as humans. They will remove seeds from food like bell peppers and skin from chickpeas (I noticed they are particularly efficient at this one). Compared to larger mammals who are often seen inhaling entire pieces of food at once, capuchin monkeys are more advanced in their methods of dealing with food.

            Perhaps one of the most rewarding aspects of interning at Living Links is the opportunity to build an understanding of the monkeys. Obviously, I am not there yet as I can barely recognise a handful of monkeys. But I hope that over time, I will be able to tell them apart, get to know each individual’s unique personality and preferences, and develop a deep appreciation for the complexity and beauty of their lives.

            I am looking forward to the rest of my internship at Living Links and I am sure this will be an eye-opening experience.

Abstract Knowledge: Uniquely Human?

Written by: Anna Redly

A recent study comparing the abilities of 4- to 5-year-old children and the Living Links capuchins found that only the human children were able to make predictions using abstract knowledge.

Imagine that there are two brown paper bags in front of you. You know that they contain sweets, but not what kind. You reach into the first bag and pull out a handful of four green gobstoppers. You then reach into the second bag and draw out four chocolate buttons. After just taking four sweets from each bag, you can make a reasonable prediction about what kind of sweet you might find if you went in for seconds in either bag: a green gobstopper for the first bag, and a chocolate button from the second bag. This is called a ‘Level 1 Abstraction’. You will also likely arrive at the conclusion that brown paper bags contain sweets of the same kind – this is called a ‘Level 2 Abstraction’. You would only need to take one sweet from a third bag to predict that all the other sweets in the bag will be the same kind.

Abstractions represent generalised information that is common across different situations, like in the sweet example. This ability is central to the way we humans navigate the world, helping us to learn quickly from limited information and make predictions in new situations. The questions that Felsche and colleagues wanted to investigate were how early human children develop this ability and whether it is an ability that we share with other primate species.

To explore this question, Felsche and colleagues designed an experiment with a similar principle to the sweet example. The researcher would sample items from three containers, which contained sticker strips for the children and food for the monkeys. There were two experimental conditions. In one condition, the sampled items supported the idea that items were sorted into containers by their type (e.g., four apple pieces from one container and four raisins from another). In the other condition, sampled items suggested that items were sorted into containers based on size (e.g., four small items of different types from one container and four large items of different types from another). After exposure to one of the two conditions, the participants were then presented with two new containers and an example item from each and were prompted to choose from which container they wanted to receive their next item. 

Diagram 1: Experimental Conditions

In both conditions, the two example items were always a small but high-value item from one container, and a large but low-value item from the other container. The prediction was, that when the children and monkeys were in the type condition, they should choose the next item from the container with the high value example item – so that they can get a high value item too. In the size condition, one high-value sample does not guarantee that the rest of the items in the container will be high-value, as items are sorted into containers based on their size rather than type. If the participants recognised this, they would choose the large and low-value option because no matter which food type will be next, they at least can secure another big item, which is always better than a smaller one.

The researchers found that the capuchins choices of test containers was at random and not influenced by whether they had previously seen that treats are sorted into containers based on their size or their type. This performance suggests that the monkeys did not learn about the abstract rules determining food distribution patterns across containers. The children, however, chose the hidden sample linked to the small high value item more often in the type condition compared to the size condition. This sensitivity to the experimental condition suggests that the children were able to generalise at the second level of abstraction and make predictions accordingly.

The researchers then designed a second experiment, to see if the capuchins were able to form Level 1 Abstractions within this paradigm. In this experiment, like in the sweet example, the monkeys and children were presented with only two containers from which four evidence items were sampled, respectively. Like in experiment 1, there were two experimental conditions (type and size), but this time the choice items were sampled directly from the original containers, so no generalisation to new containers was required.

Again, the capuchins performed at chance level in both conditions, suggesting that they are unable to form Level 1 Abstractions. The children performed above chance in the type condition, but seemingly at random in the size condition. This is interesting, implying that while children can use Level 1 and 2 Abstractions to inform predictions, this ability might depend on the item characteristic they form generalisations about (e.g. type or size). However, the researchers also ran a computational model based on the children’s and monkeys’ preferences. This model suggested that the children’s above chance performance in the type condition could be due to the fact that they simply cared more about their reward’s type than its size.

This study has extended our current knowledge of abstract knowledge formation in non-human species and provided a novel task design that can be altered to learn more about the abilities of different species – both contributing to the investigation of whether humans are indeed unique in our ability to use abstract knowledge.

Want to know more? Click here to read the paper!

What is Living Links? Click here to learn more.